Skip to main content
Log in

Rituals, Repetitiveness and Cognitive Load

A Competitive Test of Ritual Benefits for Stress

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A central hypothesis to account for the ubiquity of rituals across cultures is their supposed anxiolytic effects: rituals being maintained because they reduce existential anxiety and uncertainty. We aimed to test the anxiolytic effects of rituals by investigating two possible underlying mechanisms for it: cognitive load and repetitive movement. In our pre-registered experiment (osf.io/rsu9x), 180 undergraduates took part in either a stress or a control condition and were subsequently assigned to either control, cognitive load, undirected movement, a combination of undirected movement and cognitive load, or a ritualistic intervention. Using both repeated self-report measures and continuous physiological indicators of anxiety, we failed to find direct support for a cognitive suppression effect of anxiety through ritualistic behavior. Nevertheless, we found that induced stress increased participants’ subsequent repetitive behavior, which in turn reduced physiological arousal. This study provides novel evidence for plausible underlying effects of the proposed anxiolytic effect of rituals: repetitive behavior but not cognitive load may decrease physiological stress responses during ritual.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. To increase readability, some of our hypothesis have been renumbered; no changes were made to proposed methods or analysis.

References

  • Alcorta, C. S., & Sosis, R. (2005). Ritual, emotion, and sacred symbols. Human Nature, 16(4), 323–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anastasi, M. W., & Newberg, A. B. (2008). A preliminary study of the acute effects of religious ritual on anxiety. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 14(2), 163–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, E., & Shivakumar, G. (2013). Effects of exercise and physical activity on anxiety. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00027.

  • Bell, C. (2009). Ritual: Perspective and dimensions. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P., & Liénard, P. (2006). Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(6), 595–613.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P., & Liénard, P. (2008). Ritual behavior in obsessive and normal individuals. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(4), 291–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, A. W., Schroeder, J., Risen, J. L., Gino, F., Galinsky, A. D., Norton, M. I., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2016). Don’t stop believing: Rituals improve performance by decreasing anxiety. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 137, 71–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burdwood, E. N., Infantolino, Z. P., Crocker, L. D., Spielberg, J. M., Banich, M. T., Miller, G. A., & Heller, W. (2016). Resting-state functional connectivity differentiates anxious apprehension and anxious arousal. Psychophysiology, 53, 1451–1459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Causer, J., Holmes, P. S., Smith, N. C., & Williams, A. M. (2011). Anxiety, movement kinematics, and visual attention in elite-level performers. Emotion, 11(3), 595–602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: Psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100(3), 316–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cluff, T., Boulet, J., & Balasubramaniam, R. (2011). Learning a stick-balancing task involves task-specific coupling between posture and hand displacements. Experimental Brain Research, 213, 15–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coco, M. I., & Dale, R. (2014). Cross-recurrence quantification analysis of categorical and continuous time series: An R package. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, R., Callander, R., Reddish, P., & Bulbulia, J. (2013). How do rituals affect cooperation? Human Nature, 24(2), 115–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, R., Xygalatas, D., Mitkidis, P., Reddish, P., Tok, P., Konvalinka, I., & Bulbulia, J. (2014). The fire-walkers’ high: Affect and physiological responses in an extreme collective ritual. PLoS One, 9(2), e88355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fowles, D. C., Christie, M. J., Edelberg, R., Grings, W. W., Lykken, D. T., & Venables, P. H. (1981). Publication recommendations for electrodermal measurements. Psychophysiology, 18(3), 232–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fux, M., Eilam, D., Mort, J., Keren, H., & Lawson, E. T. (2013). Are motor collective rituals as rigid as they seem? A test case of a Zulu wedding dance. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 13(1–2), 17–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hembree, R. (1988). Correlates, causes, effects, and treatment of test anxiety. Review of Educational Research, 58(1), 47–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Higuchia, T., Imanakab, K., & Hatayamac, T. (2002). Freezing degrees of freedom under stress: Kinematic evidence of constrained movement strategies. Human Movement Science, 21, 831–846.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hirsh, J. B., Mar, R. A., & Peterson, J. B. (2012). Psychological entropy: A framework for understanding uncertainty-related anxiety. Psychological Review, 119(2), 304–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, N. M., Bonk, D., & Inzlicht, M. (2017a). Rituals decrease the neural response to performance failure. PeerJ, 5, e3363. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.3363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, N. M., Schroeder, J., Risen, J. L., Xygalatas, D., & Inzlicht, M. (2017b). The psychology of rituals: An integrative review and process-based framework. Personality and Social Psychology Review., 22, 260–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Irons, W. (1996). Morality, religion, and human nature. In W. M. Richardson & W. Wildman (Eds.), Religion and science: History, method, dialogue (pp. 375–399). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, S. H., & Rocklin, T. (1994). The temporal patterns of worry and emotionality and their differential effects on test performance. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 7(2), 117–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The Trier Social Stress Test: A tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28(1–2), 76–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kowalski, R. M. (2000). Anxiety In Encyclopaedia of Psychology. American Psychological Association.

  • Krátký, J., Lang, M., Shaver, J. H., Jerotijević, D., & Xygalatas, D. (2016). Anxiety and ritualization: Can attention discriminate compulsion from routine? Communicative & Integrative Biology, 9(3), e1174799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-8. Gainesville, FL: NIMH Center for Emotion and Attention (CSEA) at the University of Florida.

  • Lang, M., Krátký, J., Shaver, J. H., Jerotijević, D., & Xygalatas, D. (2015). Effects of anxiety on spontaneous ritualized behavior. Current Biology, 25, 1892–1897.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Legare, C. H., & Souza, A. L. (2012). Evaluating ritual efficacy: Evidence from the supernatural. Cognition, 124(1), 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malinowski, A. W. (1954). Argonauts of the western Pacific. An account of native enterprise and adventure in the archipelagoes of Melanesian New Guinea. London: George Routledge & Sons.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Marteau, T. M., & Bekker, H. (1992). The development of a six-item short-form of the state scale of the Spielberger State-trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31(3), 301–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marwan, N., Carmen Romano, M., Thiel, M., & Kurths, J. (2007). Recurrence plots for the analysis of complex systems. Physics Reports, 438, 237–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the Penn State worry questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28(6), 487–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mogan, R., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. A. (2017). To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony’s effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 13–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molina, S., & Borkovec, T. D. (1994). The Penn State worry questionnaire: Psychometric properties and associated characteristics. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nitschke, J. B., Heller, W., Palmieri, P. A., & Miller, G. A. (1999). Contrasting patterns of brain activity in anxious apprehension and anxious arousal. Psychophysiology, 36(5), 628–637.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norton, M. I., & Gino, F. (2013). Rituals alleviate grieving for loved ones, lovers, and lotteries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 266–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pfeiffer, J. E. (1982). The creative explosion: An inquiry into the origins of art and religion. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rappaport, R. (1999). Ritual and religion in the making of humanity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Renner, K.-H., Hock, M., Bergner-Köther, R., & Laux, L. (2016). Differentiating anxiety and depression: The state-trait anxiety-depression inventory. Cognition and Emotion. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1266306.

  • Ruffle, B. J., & Sosis, R. (2007). Does it pay to pray? Costly ritual and cooperation. The B.E. Journal of Economic Analysis & Policy, 7(1), 1–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmon, P. (2001). Effects of physical exercise on anxiety, depression, and sensitivity to stress: A unifying theory. Clinical Psychology Review, 21(1), 33–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seipp, B. (1991). Anxiety and academic performance: A meta-analysis of findings. Anxiety Research, 4(1), 27–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R., & Handwerker, W. P. (2011). Psalms and coping with uncertainty: Religious Israeli women’s responses to the 2006 Lebanon war. American Anthropologist, 113(1), 40–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R., & Ruffle, B. J. (2003). Religious ritual and cooperation: Testing for a relationship on Israeli religious and secular kibbutzim. Current Anthropology, 44, 713–722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vytal, K. E., Cornwell, B., Arkin, N., & Grillon, C. (2012). Describing the interplay between anxiety and cognition: From impaired performance under low cognitive load to reduced anxiety under high load. Psychophysiology, 49, 842–852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vytal, K. E., Cornwell, B. R., Letkiewicz, A. M., Arkin, N. E., & Grillon, C. (2013). The complex interaction between anxiety and cognition: Insight from spatial and verbal working memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00093.

  • Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1063–1070.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wen, N. J., Herrmann, P. A., & Legare, C. H. (2016). Ritual increases children’s affiliation with in-group members. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(1), 54–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, H. (1995). Inside the cult: Religious innovation and transmission in Papua New Guinea. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Xygalatas, D., Schjoedt, U., Bulbulia, J., Konvalinka, I., Jegindø, E.-M., Reddish, P., et al. (2013). Autobiographical memory in a fire-walking ritual. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 13(1–2), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Johannes Alfons Karl.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1

(PDF 320 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Karl, J.A., Fischer, R. Rituals, Repetitiveness and Cognitive Load. Hum Nat 29, 418–441 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-018-9325-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-018-9325-3

Keywords

Navigation